VOLUME 26 NUMBER 4 OCTOBER 1987
Articles and Notes
Plasma Cortisol of Female Rhesus Monkeys in Response to Acute Restraint, by S. W. Line, A. S. Clarke, & H, Markowitz ...... 1
Environmental Enrichment with Sticks for Singly-Caged Adult Rhesus Monkeys, by M. Champoux, M. Hempel & V. Reinhardt ...... 5
News, Information, and Announcements
Chimpanzee Committee Formed ...... 4
News Briefs ...... 8
. . . Smith Introduces Legislation to Send IBR Research Animals to Primarily Primates; State Legislatures Declare Support for Animal Research; NIH Establishes Office of Animal Care and Use; Prisoners of Science; Macaquepit Monkey Business on 747
Revision of Directory of Graduate Programs in Primatology and Primate Research Planned ...... 9
Opinion: Scarce Resources and Endangered Species ...... 10
NIH Workshop on PHS Policy ...... 11
Symposium: Transplacental Effects on Fetal Health ...... 11
Erratum ...... 26
Departments
Recent Books and Articles ...... 12
* * *
Scott W. Line, A. Susan Clarke, and Hal Markowitz
University of California, Davis, and San Francisco State University
Introduction
Primates kept in laboratory colonies experience a variety of handling and manipulative procedures in conjunction with health, breeding, or experimental protocols. Examples range from blood sampling, weighing, injections, and palpations to more invasive procedures such as C-sections. All of these necessitate some degree of restraint (manual or chemical) or confinement (such as in a transfer box) of the animal. To the extent that the restraint procedures themselves are stressful (albeit often unavoidably), they have implications for the well-being of laboratory primates, an issue which has become the subject of wide concern.
Previous studies have investigated the response of laboratory primates to several forms of restraint. The majority of these have noted behavioral and physiologic responses indicative of moderate to severe psychologic stress in response to handling and manual restraint (Mason, Harcourt, & Rosenthal, 1957), tethering (Kaplan, Adams, & Bumsted, 1983), chair restraint (Kling & Orbach, 1963; Mason, Harcourt, & Rosenthal, 1957), surgery board restraint (Clarke, Mason, & Moberg, 1987), and anesthesia (Wall, Worthman, & Else, 1985; Elvidge et al, 1976). Habituation to some procedures may occur only slowly (Burton, Burns, & Smith, 1981; Coe, et al, 1983; Kaplan, Adams, & Bumsted, 1983).
Less severe manipulations may also result in measurable stress responses, even in experienced animals. Movement to a novel cage may cause behavioral and physiologic stress responses (Coe, et al, 1983; Hennessy, Mendoza, & Kaplan, 1982; Mitchell & Gomber, 1976; Nash & Chilton, 1986), as may confinement in a transfer box for only a brief period (Clarke, 1987). At least some habituation occurs to these procedures after repeated exposure (Clarke, 1987; Coe, et al, 1983).
This investigation compared plasma cortisol responses of female rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) to short periods of manual restraint in the home cage with those obtained after short periods of confinement in a transfer box. These procedures are commonly used in primate colonies for blood-sampling (manual restraint) and management procedures (transport).
Methods
The subjects were 10 adult female rhesus macaques (age range 5-11 yrs). They were all colony-born in large outdoor multimale, multifemale groups. They had lived outdoors for at least 25 months and had been housed indoors in single cages for at least 15 months. They were housed in two rooms with approximately 50 other rhesus monkeys for five months prior to the study. The animal rooms were illuminated for 12 hours daily, and cleaned daily at 0900-1000 hours. They were fed commercial monkey chow at 0800-0900 and 1400-1500 hours. The monkeys were being used for an environmental enrichment experiment which evaluated their behavioral and physiologic responses to a cage enrichment apparatus (a device which delivered food and music). They were assigned to experimental (apparatus present) and control (no apparatus) groups of 5 monkeys each. During this time the monkeys had experienced brief manual restraint for venipuncture at two week intervals. The monkeys had experienced brief transfer box confinement approximately 4-5 times a year during the time they were housed indoors.
Tests of the monkeys' plasma cortisol responses to restraint in the home cage and confinement in a transfer box were performed at two week intervals. Each monkey was tested twice in the home cage (tests 1 and 2) and then twice in the transfer box (tests 3 and 4). All tests were performed between 1300 and 1600 hours. Pre-test blood samples were collected within three minutes of entering the animal room in all tests. Venipuncture was accomplished by restraining the monkey using the cage squeeze mechanism and withdrawing the arm through the partially open cage door. For the home cage test the monkey was then held in this position for 15 minutes, after which a post-test blood sample was taken. For the transfer box test the subject was placed for five minutes in a transfer box that remained attached to the front of the cage. The monkey was then returned to its cage and the experimenters left the room. They returned 15 minutes later and collected a post-test blood sample, again within three minutes of entering the room. Thus, for both conditions, post-test sample results reflect not only the response to restraint, but also to the pre-test venipuncture.
All blood samples were collected into syringes containing Na EDTA. Samples were placed on ice and centrifuged at 4deg C within 20 minutes of collection. Plasma was removed and stored at -70deg C until assayed for cortisol via radioimmunoassay. The intraassay coefficient of variation was 4.3%.
The mean plasma cortisol levels of the experimental and control groups were compared by a Student's t-test. None of the differences between means were significant, thus the data for both groups were combined for subsequent analysis. Pre- and post-test cortisol levels were then compared with a 2-way analysis of variance for each condition.
Results
There was a nonsignificant (p = .21) increase in mean plasma cortisol levels in response to venipuncture and restraint in the home cage (Figure 1). Some of the monkeys struggled briefly during the restraint, but most sat calmly throughout the 15 minute period. In contrast, the mean increase in cortisol in response to venipuncture and confinement in a transfer box was highly significant (p = .003). Several monkeys urinated or defecated in the transfer box, or displayed submissive postures toward the experimenters. The mean cortisol levels for both the pre- and post-test samples were lower in test 4 than in test 3 (p = .0243), but the magnitude of the increase in cortisol between samples was similar in both tests.
Figure 1: Mean (+/-SE) plasma cortisol levels of 10 female rhesus macaques before and after 15 minutes of restraint in the home cage (tests 1 and 2) and 5 minutes of confinement in a transfer box (tests 3 and 4).
Discussion
The lack of an increase in cortisol in response to restraint in the home cage was an unexpected finding. Physical immobilization has often been found to cause a significant physiologic response in experimental animals. While the monkeys in this experiment had never been restrained for 15 minutes prior to the first home cage restraint test, they had been restrained for three minutes for blood sampling at two week intervals 10 times before the experiment. Thus they were familiar with the experimenters and the blood sampling process, and the only new component in tests 1 and 2 was the duration of restraint. The results indicate that the increase in the duration of restraint was not a significant event to the monkeys.
Confinement in a transfer box was a significant event, as measured by cortisol response, even though this condition was presumably intrinsically less stressful than manual restraint. Transfer box confinement represents a reduction in space from the home cage and thus some restriction in movement, but to a much lesser degree than the near-complete immobilization accomplished by use of the cage squeeze mechanism. Moreover, duration of confinement was less than for home cage restraint (five vs fifteen minutes). Both duration and degree of restraint have been correlated with magnitude of cortisol response in macaques (Clarke, Mason, & Moberg, 1987). All of the monkeys had been in transfer boxes before, but not for 6 months prior to tests 3 and 4. While it presumably caused no physical discomfort, box confinement presented a change from the normal routine. The pronounced cortisol response to box confinement but not to home cage restraint suggests that the salient difference between the two conditions for the monkeys was a difference in the relative novelty of the two situations. However, placement in a transfer box is often a prelude to other stressful events such as movement to a new cage or experimental procedures, and association with such events may also have contributed to the monkeys' responses.
There was no clear evidence of habituation of the monkeys' response to the transfer box. The pre- and post-test cortisol levels were lower in test 4 than test 3, but the magnitude of the increase was approximately the same in both tests. The lower pre-test level in test 4 probably reflected normal day-to-day variation in baseline secretion of cortisol.
Finally, it should be noted that venipuncture itself did not elicit a significant cortisol response, even though such a response has been reported in other studies (Riley, 1981). In fact, the reported rapidity of this response is the basis for the common practice of obtaining blood samples within a brief and predetermined time period in studies of plasma cortisol responses to stimuli. Apparently the monkeys had habituated to the venipuncture procedure, such that this event, even with added restraint duration, did not elicit the predicted substantial cortisol response.
The results of this experiment suggest that routine events, even ones like venipuncture and restraint that involve mild physical discomfort and psychologic stress, do not cause a significant physiologic response in habituated rhesus monkeys. In contrast, other events which cause no physical discomfort may lead to a significant physiologic response if they are not part of the normal routine, even if the monkeys are familiar with the procedure. These findings agree with those from previous studies (Hennessy, Mendoza, & Kaplan, 1982; Mitchell & Gomber, 1976; Nash & Chilton, 1986; Phoenix & Chambers, 1984) indicating that novelty alone can be a significant source of stress for laboratory primates.
References
Burton, R. R., Burns, J. W., & Smith, A. H. (1981). Restraint of animals in space research. The Physiologist, 24[6], suppl., 41-44.
Clarke, A. S. (1987). Species differences in responses of macaques to transport cage training. American Journal of Primatology, 12, 336. (Abstract)
Clarke, A. S., Mason, W. A., & Moberg, G. P. (1987). Differential behavioral and adrenocortical responses to stress among three macaque species. American Journal of Primatology, (in press).
Coe, C. L., Glass, J. C., Wiener, S. G., & Levine, S. (1983). Behavioral, but not physiological, adaptation to repeated separation in mother and infant primates. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 8[4], 401-409.
Elvidge, H., Challis, J. R. G., Robinson, J. S., Roper, C., & Thorburn, G. D. (1976). Influence of handling and sedation on plasma cortisol in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Journal of Endocrinology, 70, 325-326.
Hennessy, M. B., Mendoza, S. P., & Kaplan, J. R. (1982). Behavior and plasma cortisol following brief peer separation in juvenile squirrel monkeys. American Journal of Primatology, 3, 143-151.
Kaplan, J. R., Adams, M. R., & Bumsted, P. (1983). Heart rate changes associated with tethering of cynomolgus monkeys. Laboratory Animal Science, 38[5], 493.
Kling, A. & Orbach J. (1963). Plasma 17-hydrocorticosteroid levels in the stump-tailed monkey and two other macaques. Psychological Reports, 13, 863-865.
Mason, J. W., Harcourt, C. T., & Rosenthal, J. W. (1957). Influence of some environmental factors on plasma and urinary 17-hydroxycorticosteroid levels in the rhesus monkey. American Journal of Physiology, 190, 429-433.
Mitchell, G. & Gomber, J. (1976). Moving laboratory rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) to unfamiliar home cages. Primates, 17, 543-547.
Nash, L. T. & Chilton, S. M. (1986). Space or novelty? Effects of altered cage size on Galago behavior. American Journal of Primatology, 6, 37-49.
Phoenix, C. H. & Chambers, K. C. (1984). Sexual and serum hormone levels in aging rhesus males: Effects of environmental change. Hormones and Behavior, 18, 206-215.
Riley, V. (1981). Psychoneuroendocrine influences on immunocompetence and neoplasia. Science, 212, 1100-1109.
Wall, H. S., Worthman, C., & Else, J. G. (1985). Effects of ketamine anaesthesia, stress and repeated bleeding on the haematology of vervet monkeys. Laboratory Animals, 19[2], 138-144.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
First author's address: California Primate Research Center, University of California, Davis, CA 95616.
This project was supported by NIH Grant RR00169. A. S. Clarke was supported by an NIMH post-doctoral traineeship from the Health Psychology Program, University of California, San Francisco. The authors thank Dr. Dave Hess, Oregon Regional Primate Research Center, Beaverton, OR, for performing the cortisol assays.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
* * *
The newly formed Committee for Conservation and Care of Chimpanzees (CCCC) was conceived as a loose international coalition of scientists and other professionals who conduct noninvasive research with chimpanzees and who are dedicated to their protection in the wild and to the improvement of their living conditions in captivity. At an international symposium devoted to "Understanding Chimpanzees" held on November 7 to 11, 1986, which was sponsored by the Chicago Academy of Sciences, some 30 experts expressed an interest in permanent membership. The expected total membership after a year of expansion will be about 50-60 professionals. The CCCC operates under the sponsorship of the Jane Goodall Institute for Wildlife Research, Education and Conservation, which in turn is administered by the William M. Clements Foundation.
The CCCC's most important role will be to formulate, produce, and disseminate substantive guidelines for better protection and superior care of chimpanzees. Other functions include the establishment of an information resource base on chimpanzee research, and of a special fund to survey remnant populations in Africa. Additional long-range goals remain to be defined when membership has been expanded.
Dr. Geza Teleki, who has been professionally involved with chimpanzee conservation and care issues for many years, will serve as ad hoc Chairman for a formative period of at least two years, in a full-time post devoted to organizing an international membership, establishing productive long-range objectives, and initiating formal action on priority tasks identified at the symposium. Correspondence concerning the CCCC may be sent to Dr Teleki at 3819 48th Street, NW, Washington, DC 20016 [Telephone: 202-362-1993].
* * *
Maribeth Champoux, Michael Hempel and Viktor Reinhardt
University of Wisconsin
Introduction
Legislation will soon make it imperative that individuals working with captive nonhuman primates devise methods of environmental enrichment for their research subjects. The primary rationale of these new regulations will be to enhance the psychological well-being of the captive primates. Research paradigms have been utilized both in research laboratories and zoos to examine various enrichment methodologies. These studies strongly suggest that environmental enrichment has beneficial effects on the behavior of captive primates.
Numerous studies have examined the effects of environmental enrichment on the behavior of group-housed primates. Enrichment decreases the frequencies both of self-directed and stereotypy maladaptive behavior patterns (Pfeiffer & Koebner, 1978; Clarke et al., 1982; Bloomstrand et al., 1987), and reduces aggression (Chamove et al., 1982; Maple & Stine, 1982). A particularly robust effect following environmental enrichment is increased motor activity and object manipulation (Pfeiffer & Koebner, 1978; White, 1978; Chamove et al., 1982; Clarke et al., 1982; Maple & Stine, 1982; Wilson, 1982; Tripp, 1985; Westergaard & Fragaszy, 1985). Environmental enrichment has also been found to decrease fearful or distressed behavior in novel environments (Champoux et al., 1987).
Relatively little research has focused on the problem of environmental enrichment for singly-caged animals. The addition of social partners has been utilized as an enrichment technique with success (e.g. Reinhardt et al., 1987a) but this method is not feasible in research paradigms stipulating individual housing of animals. Attempts at enrichment utilizing inanimate objects generally involve relatively expensive devices such as electrical or mechanical manipulanda which produce response-contingent feedback, food puzzles, television sets, and radios (Spinelli & Markowitz, 1985; Line, 1987; Clarke et al., 1987), though Renquist & Judge (1985) used nylon balls as an enrichment technique. Reinhardt and co-workers (1987b) found that simply providing singly-caged rhesus macaques with a deciduous tree branch proved equally effective in enriching the animals' barren environment.
In this study the cages of singly-housed adult rhesus macaques were enriched by the addition of small sticks from deciduous trees. Sticks were chosen because they are inexpensive, readily obtainable, easy to clean, and, unlike branches, are usable in squeeze cages as well as in standard cages.
Methods
Subjects were 48 randomly selected singly-caged adult rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). There were 28 females, 13 males, and 7 pseudohermaphrodite females ranging in age from 6 to 30 years. Individuals were housed in 70 x 75 x 77 cm stainless steel cages in rooms maintained on a 12:12 light cycle. The animals were fed Purina Monkey Chow between 0700 and 0800; water was available ad lib. All animals were checked daily for health assessment.
Each animal was provided with a stick for a 12-week period. For the first two weeks of the study, sticks were cut from felled box elder trees (Acer negundo). The sticks were 19 cm long and had a mean radius of 2.5 cm, and a mean volume of 370 cc. In the third week of the study, the box elder sticks were replaced with thick black cherry sticks (Prunus serotina). The substitution was made because the soft box elder wood produced long thick splinters when gnawed by the monkeys; these frequently clogged drains. The situation was alleviated with the hard black cherry wood. The black cherry sticks were also 19 cm long, but they had a mean radius of 4.5 cm and a mean volume of 1200 cc. A black cherry stick was replaced whenever it was worn to half its original volume. To assess use of the sticks, their volumes were measured every two weeks. A stick was considered to have been used if either of the two following criteria were met:
1) if the reduction in the volume of the stick was greater than 2% every two weeks, or:
2) if the reduction in the volume of the stick was less than 2%, but the animal was observed or heard rolling the stick over the floor at least twice every two weeks.
Behavioral observations were conducted on 13 laboratory-born monkeys ranging in age from 5 to 25 years. They were initially habituated to the presence of one observer (the senior author) during a two-week period. Eight baseline behavioral observations were conducted after this familiarization period over a 3-week block prior to enrichment. Five further observations were carried out during the initial 10 days following the introduction of the stick. All observations were conducted between 1530 and 1630 on weekdays. Only the initial behavior(s) exhibited by the animals was recorded.
For the purposes of analysis, the observed behaviors were combined into the following categories: sitting posture; standing or crouching posture; activity (environmental exploration, locomotion, or eating); self-directed; and hostility to observer. The percent of observations in which the animal was observed engaging in each behavior category was calculated for both the preenrichment and postenrichment phases. Pre- and postenrichment data were compared utilizing a one-way analysis of variance.
Results
The animals used their sticks in 65% (31/48) of cases; of the animals that used their sticks, 84% (26/31) used them continually throughout the 12 weeks, whereas 16% (5/31) stopped using them after 2 to 6 weeks. Gnawing accounted for 90% (28/31) of stick usage, rolling for only 10% (3/31). Figure 1 depicts two typical examples of the patterns of stick use during the 12 week study period.
Figure 1: Example of two-singly caged adult rhesus monkeys who continually used their sticks for gnawing throughout the 12-week study.
When all animals are entered into the analysis regardless of use of stick, the cage enrichment had a significant effect on self-directed behavior (F(1,13) = 8.43, p = .01). Self-directed behavior was observed in 23.5 percent of scoring sessions prior to enrichment, versus 11.4 percent postenrichment. Prior to enrichment, the animals could be divided into those which exhibited activity and those which did not. Enrichment had the effect of increasing the level of activity of the second category of animals only, from 0 percent to 22.9 percent. The activity rates of the first category of animals remained relatively constant at 17.3 percent preenrichment and 17.5 percent postenrichment. Other behavior categories were not significantly affected by the introduction of the stick.
Discussion
Addition of sticks to the home cages of singly-housed adult rhesus macaques was found to be an effective and inexpensive method of environmental enrichment. The sticks were utilized by the majority of the subject population, with only 16% of them gradually losing interest over the twelve-week test period. Enrichment decreased the frequency of self-directed behavior, but increased the activity levels of those animals which had previously been inactive. These results concur with previous studies of enrichment on group-housed animals (Pfeiffer & Koebner, 1978; Clarke et al., 1982; Bloomstrand et al., 1987), and suggest that the well-being of singly-caged animals may be enhanced in many instances by a relatively simple environmental manipulation.
It is undeniable that environmental enrichment for singly-caged primates is vitally important, as these animals are generally denied the social stimulation allowed to group-housed animals. Because the research protocol of many laboratories will continue to call for singly-caged animals, it is essential that more paradigms be developed that test potential forms of enrichment.
References
Bloomstrand, M., Alford, P. L., & Maple, T. L. (1987). An analysis of feeding enrichment for captive chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Paper presented at 1987 meeting of American Society of Primatologists, Madison, WI.
Chamove, A. L., Anderson, J. R., Morgan-Jones, S. C., & Jones, S. P. (1982). Deep woodchip litter: Hygiene, feeding, and behavioral enhancement in eight primate species. International Journal for the Study of Animal Problems, 3, 308-319.
Champoux, M., Schneider, M., & Suomi, S. J. (1987). Environmental enrichment and affective responses in infant rhesus macaques. Paper presented at 1987 biennial meeting of Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore, MD, and at 1987 meeting of American Society of Primatologists, Madison, WI.
Clarke, A. S., Juno, C. J., & Maple, T. L. (1982). Behavioral effects of a change in the physical environment: A pilot study of captive chimpanzees. Zoo Biology, 1, 371-380.
Clarke, A. S., Line, S., Ellman, G., & Markowitz, H. (1987). Hormonal and behavioral responses of rhesus macaques to an environmental enrichment apparatus. Paper presented at 1987 meeting of American Society of Primatologists, Madison, WI.
Line, S. W. (1987). Environmental enrichment for laboratory primates. Journal of the American Veterinary Medicine Association, 190, 854-859.
Maple, T. L., & Stine, W. W. (1982). Environmental variables and great ape husbandry. American Journal of Primatology Supplement, 1, 67-76.
Pfeiffer, A. J., & Koebner, L. J. (1978). The resocialization of single-caged chimpanzees and the establishment of an island colony. Journal of Medical Primatology, 7, 70-81.
Reinhardt, V., Houser, W. D., Cowley, D., & Champoux, M. (1987a). Preliminary comments on environmental enrichment with branches for individually caged rhesus monkeys. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 26[1], 1-3.
Reinhardt, V., Cowley, D., Eisele, S., Vertein, R., & Houser, D. (1987b). Preliminary comments on pairing unfamiliar female rhesus monkeys for the purpose of environmental enrichment. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 26[2], 5-8.
Renquist, D. M., & Judge, F. J. (1985). Use of nylon balls as behavioral modifiers for caged primates. Laboratory Primate Newsletter, 24[4], 4.
Spinelli, J. S., & Markowitz, H. (1985). Prevention of cage-associated distress. Lab Animal, 14, 19-28.
Tripp, J. K. (1985). Increasing activity in captive orangutans: Provision of manipulatable and edible material. Zoo Biology, 4, 225-234.
Westergaard, G. C., & Fragaszy, D. M. (1985). Effects of manipulatable objects on the activity of captive capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). Zoo Biology, 4, 317-327.
White, L. Behavioral response of orangutans to remodeling of their exhibit. (1978). Animal Keepers Forum, 5, 179-180.
Wilson, S. F. (1982). Environmental influences on the activity of captive apes. Zoo Biology, 1, 201-209.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
Authors' address: Wisconsin Regional Primate Research Center, 1223 Capitol Court, Madison, WI 53715.
We are grateful to Ms. Linda Endlich for preparing Figure 1.
The project was supported by NIH grant RR00167 to the Wisconsin Regional Primate Research Center.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
* * *
Smith Introduces Legislation to Send IBR Research Animals to Primarily Primates
As promised at a June 25 rally held on the Capitol steps, Rep. Robert C. Smith (R-NH) has introduced H.R. 2883, a bill "to provide for the transfer of certain monkeys to the animal sanctuary known as Primarily Primates, Inc." The legislation directs the Secretary of Health and Human Services to take such steps as are necessary to enter into an agreement with Primarily Primates, Inc. The agreement is for the transfer of fifteen primates which were purchased with funds from an NIH grant to the Institute for Behavioral Research related to the effects of somatosensory deafferentation. The animals are currently in the custody of NIH and most are being cared for at the Delta Regional Primate Center in Louisiana.
The terms of the agreement outlined in H.R. 2883 are that Primarilv Primates shall not transfer ownership or possession of the animals to any other party or allow any research to be conducted on such animals, other than research which is noninvasive and observational in nature. Primarily Primates is to "reimburse the U.S. for any expense relating to the maintainence of the animals ... incurred by the U.S. after the date of such transfer." The bill states it is not to be considered "to limit any right of the U.S. to recover expenses associated with the animals from a person who received, or used funds provided by" the grant referred to above.
When introduced on July 1, the bill had 6 co-sponsors, Hamilton Fish (R-NY), Frank Horton (R-NY), Tom Lantos (D-CA), Charlie Rose (D-NC), Patricia Schroeder (D-CO), and Gus Yatron (D-PA). As of September 8, there were 31 co-sponsors.
Concerned primatologists may write to their Representatives in Congress and express their opinion of this proposed legislation.
Meanwhile, on September 1, five untreated male cynomolgus control animals, accompanied by a veterinarian, were flown to the San Diego Zoo. According to a San Diego Zoo spokesperson, the animals arrived safely and will begin a re-socialization period of about one year before being placed in a smaller zoo elsewhere in California. The other nine animals involved in the IBR project will remain at Delta. A female rhesus monkey, another control, is now progressing rapidly toward full socialization with a male rhesus cagemate. The remaining eight macaques, all of which underwent surgical deafferentation during the IBR study, are doing as well as can be expected given serious and progressive spine abnormalities. Any attempt at socialization involving vigorous physical activity will put them at risk for broken necks or backs. Each will continue to require individual care until natural death, or, in conformance with animal welfare regulations, euthanasia is necessary. The Louisiana SPCA, in a report requested by the Governor, conducted a full review of the animals' health status and has recommended the animals be euthanized. However, no decision is expected before January.
Private funds covered the San Diego trip and will continue to defray the cost of care and maintenance of the animals at Delta.
State Legislatures Declare Support for Animal Research
The National Conference of State Legislatures has issued the following official policy concerning the use of animals in scientific research.
"The National Conference of State Legislatures firmly supports the use of federal, state and local authority to ensure the humane treatment of all animals. At the same time, the Conference recognizes the vital contribution made by animals to medical and scientific research. Without the use of animals in this research, few doubt that information critical to the identification of the causes or the cures for many life threatening diseases and injuries could not have been found or could have only been discovered after much longer delays and greater human suffering.
"We urge the Congress and the Administration, therefore, to exercise extreme caution in adopting any additional laws or regulations which would prohibit the use of animals in scientific or medical research or would impose such conditions as to make the cost of utilization prohibitive."
NIH Establishes Office of Animal Care and Use
The National Institutes of Health has established an Office of Animal Care and Use (OACU) to ensure that its intramural programs are in compliance with all Federal policies and rules on animal care and use. Dr. Robert A. Whitney, Jr., director of the NIH Division of Research Services and chief veterinary officer of the U.S. Public Health Service Commissioned Corps, has been appointed director of OACU, which is responsible to the NIH deputy director for intramural research, Dr. Joseph E. Rall.
Two veterinarian specialists in laboratory animal medicine are on the staff; Dr. Thomas L. Wolfle is deputy director and Dr. Martin Morin is associate director for assurance. They work closely with the NIH Animal Care and Use Committee, chaired by Dr. Whitney.
OACU also administers the Interagency Research Animal Committee, chaired by Dr. Whitney, which advises the Assistant Secretary for Health on animal-related matters affecting Federal programs. It serves as a liaison with other Federal agencies that sponsor or regulate the use of animals in research, testing, or training, as well as with international organizations on matters of animal care and use. A primary role of this committee is to foster the development of federally coordinated animal care and use policies within each participant agency.
Prisoners of Science
Jane Goodall published an article in the New York Times magazine on May 24, 1987, titled "Prisoners of Science: Chimpanzees in Medical Research." In it she called for better care and more understanding to improve the quality of life for chimpanzees used in necessary medical research. She writes, "Without doubt it will be considerably more costly to maintain chimpanzees in [this manner]. Should we begrudge them the extra dollars? We take from them their freedom, their health and often, ultimately, their lives. Surely the least we can do is to try to provide them with some of the things which could make their imprisonment more bearable."
Macaquepit Monkey Business on 747
A monkey slipped out of its cage aboard a China Airlines cargo plane [before landing] at Kennedy Airport, forced the crew from the cockpit, and played with the controls before it was nabbed. "He wasn't making any demands--he wasn't trying to take the plane back home," said John Schneider, an animal control officer from the Port Authority who netted the 15-pound macaque monkey after a 90-minute game of hide-and-seek through the 747 jet. (The monkey had crawled out of the cargo area and into the flight deck.)--[UPI, in San Fancisco Chronicle, July 31, 1987.]
* * *
.
A revised Directory will be published in the January, 1988, issue of
the
Laboratory Primate Newsletter. If you wish to have your program
listed or to revise your entry, please send us the necessary
information, following the format shown here as closely as possible.
Return the information as soon as possible, but not later than December
15, 1987, to Mrs. Janice Viticonte, Psychology Dept., Brown University,
Providence, RI 02912.
Please note that the Directory is not intended for post-doctoral
programs, though any such sent to us will be listed separately.
* * *
Michael E. Soulé
Smithsonian Institution
A few flagship species are, today, receiving the lion's share of the funding. Is all the attention on a handful of endangered species really justified? I am often asked this question by sincere conservationists who are concerned with the apparent favoritism that is lavished on a few charismatic or cuddly animals, such as the peregrine falcon, the California condor, the black-footed ferret, the giant panda, the various rhinos, and other large vertebrates.
My usual, pat answers went something like this: I don't see a real problem with singling out a few species for special attention. Granted, it is undemocratic, perhaps even feudalistic, to grant special stays and dispensations to a few courtier species that happen to enjoy our condescension. But, we must look at the alternatives, and try to enumerate the advantages and disadvantages of this interspecific cronyism.
One justification for the present system, the "keystone principle," is that the favored species are often large animals, and therefore are keystone predators of major herbivores. There is increasing evidence that their disappearance from a community can initiate serious ripple effects. The propagation of these effects can lead to major changes in the interaction of species, and to disappearances of entire guilds such as ant-following and ground-nesting birds in the tropics and early successional stages (such as those created by browsing).
A second argument, also ecological, is the "umbrella argument;" it goes like this. Yes, the tiger and grizzley get special attention, but look at the benefits. These large generalist animals require a lot of space, and lots of habitat diversity. In addition, if our objective is to make a park or nature reserve that is large enough to hold a minimum viable population of these species, it must be huge--thousands of square kilometers in most cases. Therefore, a "trickle-down" effect of these space-intensive species is to provide a lot of habitat for other, less attractive species--species that need protection, but which lack sex appeal.
An implied assumption made by those who would support a more egalitarian allocation of scarce conservation funds is that if the funds were not spent on condors or tigers, they would be available for kangaroo rats, lizards, and rare plants and insects. Is it true? Often it isn't. It is a principle of fund raising that money that is given for one cause is not necessarily available for other, similar causes. This could be called the "niche theory of giving." That is, the people who contribute to Greenpeace to save baby seals and to the WWF to save pandas, won't give to buy hectarage of tropical forests or to support research on endangered snakes, and vice versa. The implication is that there isn't much we can do about democratizing conservation. So, we might as well spend the money on those species that are lucky enough to attract it.
Another principle of fund raising tends to soften the blow, however. This could be called the "cornucopia rule"--there is plenty of money out there; it is simply a matter of identifying the "universe" of donors who are interested in your cause, and then making the correct pitch. Indeed there are societies to save endangered bats, fishes, and insects.
Adding all this up, it was easy to convince myself that one shouldn't be alarmed by conservation elitism. But, now, I'm not so sure. Let's re-examine the preceding arguments.
First, there are many species that don't profit from the keystone principle. The reason is that many charismatic species are really not ecological keystones. For example, the disappearance of condors, peregrines, whooping cranes, and even grizzleys from many regions would have very little implact that could not be dampened by counterpart species. Second, the trickle-down or umbrella effect, the noblesse oblige of animal emperors, really doesn't confer benefits on many smaller, threatened taxa. Many threatened species are habitat specialists whose ranges don't happen to coincide with those of their charismatic "benefactors."
Third, there are many species that require more than just space--they need special management interventions such as improved nesting sites, or denning sites. Other species must depend on management for the control of diseases, control of competitive or predatory exotic species, control of hydrological variables, or protection from humans. The world is so disturbed in many places that mere benign neglect is of little or no help.
Fourth, the cornucopia rule has only limited validity. It fails altogether in government agencies where funds are definitely finite, and where money spent on species A diminishes the pool of dollars for species B, C, and D. This can be a source of great anguish for many dedicated biologists in state and federal agencies, who must take from B in order to give to A, and who must risk the alienation of their colleagues in the process.
So, I am not so flippant about the supposed benefits and inevitability of our current caste system. But I don't know what to do about it.
One of the unspoken rules for the essayist is always to end on a positive, constructive note. I'd like to, but it would be premature. Perhaps it is time, though, to turn up the volume on this dialogue.--[From Endangered Species, Technical Bulletin Reprint, 4(2,3), 5.]
-------------------------------------------------------------------
Michael Soulé is the president of the Society for Conservation Biology. He is currently a fellow in Biology at the Smithsonian Instution, Washington, D.C. 20560.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
* * *
The National Institutes of Health, Office for Protection from Research Risks, is continuing to sponsor workshops on implementing the Public Health Service Policy on the Humane Care and Use of Laboratory Animals. The workshops are open to institutional administrators, members of animal care and use committees, laboratory animal veterinarians, investigators, and other institutional staff who have responsibility for high-quality management of sound institutional animal care and use programs. One workshop will be held on January 28-29, 1988, at Albuquerque, NM. Contact Ms Rynda Gibbs, University of New Mexico School of Medicine, Continuing Medical Education, 815 Vassar N.E., Albuquerque, NM 87131 [Phone 505-277-3942].
* * *
A symposium on transplacental effects on fetal health will be held at the National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland on November 5-6, 1987. The purpose of this symposium is to present our current understanding of the mechanisms of action of biologic agents, the effects of disturbed maternal metabolism, and drug and chemical abuse on the developing fetus of humans and animals. Four major topics are: 1) Congenital Viral Infections 2) Congenital Bacterial and Other Infections 3) Metabolic, Drug and Chemical Teratogens 4) Genetic Engineering.
The symposium will be open and free-of-charge, and places will be reserved for at least 100 participants. Invitations will be given on a first-come, first-served basis. For further information contact George Migaki, DVM, Registry of Comparative Pathology, Armed Forces Institute of Pathology, Washington, D.C. 20306-6000; phone (202)576-2452.
* * *
Books
Apes of the World: Their Social Behavior, Communication,
Mentality and Ecology. Russell H. Tuttle, Park Ridge, NJ:
Noyes Publications, 1986. 405 pp. [Price: $55]
. . .A comprehensive synthesis of results from research on apes
since the classic work,
The Great Apes, by Robert M. and Ada Yerkes in 1929.
Contents: 1. Terminology, taxonomy, distribution, and phylogeny.
2. Positional behavior. 3. Feeding behavior. 4. Lodge sites and
nesting. 5. Tool behavior 6. Brains and mentality. 7. Communication.
8. Sociality and sociobiology. 9. Synoptic comparison of apes.
Health Benefits of Animal Research. William I. Gay (Ed.).
Washington, DC: Foundation for Biomedical Research, 1986. 82 pp.
[Paperbound. Price: $7.50 plus $2.50 postage and handling.]
. . .It is the intent of the authors of the various sections of
this report to convey to the reader a sense of the value of
biomedical research as well as the necessity for the use of animals
in that research. It was prepared as a background document for the
scientist or medical writer who seeks to improve the public's
understanding of medical research. There are chapters on the mouse,
the rabbit, the rat, the cat, and the dog, as well as one by
Frederick A. King and Cathy J. Yarbrough, titled "Medical and
Behavioral Benefits from Primate Research." That chapter has
sections on atherosclerosis, aging, reproductive behavior, emotional
expression, communication, infectious diseases, cancer, brain
research, reproduction, surgery, dentistry and environmental health.
Official Lists and Indices of Names and Works in Zoology. International Commission on Zoological Nomenclature. 1987. [Price: $110 from the Amer. Assn. Zoo. Nomenclature, c/o NHB Stop 163, Nat. Mus. Natural History, Washington, DC 20560. 60 pounds from the Internat. Trust Zoo. Nomenclature, British Mus. (Natural History), Cromwell Road, London SW7 5BD.]
The Biomedical Investigator's Handbook
for Researchers Using Animal Models.
Washington, D.C.: Foundation for Biomedical Research, 1987. 86 pp.
[Paperbound. Price: $10]
. . .This handbook reviews major ideas, techniques, and procedures
related to the use of animals in the laboratory, including minimizing
animal pain and distress, proper surgical techniques, choosing
appropriate methods of euthanasia, and aspects of animal care and
handling that can, if overlooked, ruin experimental results. Also
included are the latest rules and regulations governing animal research,
and a section devoted to the impact of the animal rights movement. There
are also guidelines and recommendations on working with journalists,
refuting the claims of animal rights advocates, and projecting a
positive image to the public.
Bibliographies
Animal Models of Toxicant Pharmacokinetics: Species Differences of Nonhuman to Human Primates or Other Animals: A Bibliography, 1979-1987. B. Caminiti. Seattle: Primate Information Center, 1987. 17 pp., (171 citations, primate index). [Price: $7.50, plus $3.00 per invoice. Send order to Primate Information Center, Regional Primate Research Center SJ-50, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195.] An update (1983-87, 57 citations) in loose pages is available at $3.00 only for those customers who purchased the 1984 edition.
Environmental Enrichment for Captive Nonhuman Primates: A Bibliography: 1972-1987. J. B. Williams. Seattle: Primate Information Center, 1987. 5 pp. (57 citations). [Price: $4.50, plus $3.00 per invoice. Ordering information same as above.]
Reports
REP: Annual Report 1986. Rijswick, The Netherlands: Division
for Health Research TNO, 1986.
. . .This is the annual report of the REP Institutes, which stands
for the Radiobiological Institute TNO, Institute for Experimental
Gerontology TNO, and Primate Center TNO, Rijswick Z.H., The
Netherlands. Of the many short notes describing the accomplishments
of the organization, the following are concerned with primates:
IMMUNOLOGY AND TRANSPLANTATION BIOLOGY. Sequential administration of
CD4 specific monoclonal antibodies in rhesus monkeys, by M. Jonker
and J. A. H. M. den Brok. T-cell specific monoclonal antibodies
abolish the blood transfusion effect on kidney allograft survival in
rhesus monkeys, by G. den Butter, M. Jonker, F. J. M. Nooij, R.
van Schilfgaarde and A. A. van Es. EXPERIMENTAL HEMATOLOGY AND CELL
BIOLOGY. Purification of rhesus monkey haemopoietic stem cells, by
J. J. Wielenga, W. R. Gerritsen, G. Wagemaker and D. W. van Bekkum.
MICROBIOLOGY. Hepatitis delta infection in chimpanzees, by A. Kos,
M. Dubbeld, A. de Reus, J. Wubben and H. Schellekens. Infection of
rhesus monkeys with a type D retrovirus causing an immunodeficiency
syndrome, by W. van Vreeswijk, H. Niphuis, M. A. Dubbeld, L. J.
Lowenstine and P. A. J. Bentvelzen. Antibodies to the human
immunodeficiency virus in infected chimpanzees, by W. H. Koornstra,
J. T. Overdevest, R. H. Dubbes, R. van den Akker, A. A. van Es and
P. A. J. Bentvelzen. Antiviral activity of a human interferon
analogue in rhesus monkeys, by A. H. Borman, P. H. van der Meide and
H. Schellekens. ETHOLOGY. Alcohol drinking by rhesus monkeys:
Abstinence induced "catch-up" drinking is mediated by opioid
receptors, by M. Kornet, C. Goosen, L. G. Ribbens and J. M. van Ree.
Housing conditions and the ontogeny of stereotyped locomotion and
abnormal self-directed activities in rhesus monkeys, by C. Goosen.
Primate Report, No. 16, March, 1987. [Published in
cooperation with the German Primate Center (DPZ).] [Price:
$8.00]
. . .This issue includes some of the Proceedings of the XIth
Congress of the International Primate Society, 20-25 July, 1986,
Göttingen, FRG. The papers published here are: Neuroendocrine
control of sexual initiating behaviour in the female common marmoset
(Callithrix jacchus), by A. F. Dixson. Influence of female
behavior and endocrine status on sexual initiation in rhesus monkey
groups, by J. G. Herdon, M. C. Ruiz de Elvira and J. J. Turner.
Sexual initiation in stumptail macaques
(Macaca arctoides), by D. Q. Estep. Sexual initiation in common
chimpanzee, gorilla and orang-utan, by R. D. Nadler. Sexual initiation
in captive group of pygmy chimpanzees
(Pan paniscus), by J. F. Dahl. Semantic
and pragmatic meaning: Toward the analysis of meaning in primate
signs, by J. Liska. Aunting, alloparenting, infant handling:
Evolving views of adaptive function, by D. Quiatt. A preliminary
clinical investigation using simple urological techniques in
determinating urinary tract disease in a colony of
Callitrichidae, by T. J. Gatesman.
Booklets
Veterinary Biologics: Use and Regulation.
Hyattsville, MD: USDA-APHIS-VS, 1987. 8pp. [Free. Order from Public
Awareness, USDA-APHIS-LPAS, 700 Federal Building, Hyattsville, MD
20782.]
. . .This booklet explains the many uses of veterinary biologics.
Anatomy
A sternal gland in the siamang gibbon
(Hylobates syndactylus). Geissmann, T.
(Anthropological Institute, University Zürich -Irchel,
Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zürich, Switzerland.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 1-15.
. . .The present report gives the first description of a sternal
gland in the siamang gibbon, and the first histological
examination of a sternal gland in a hominoid primate other than the
orangutan.
Interrelation between ischium, thigh extending muscles and
locomotion in some primates. Yirga, S. (Primate Research Institute,
Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, 484 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 79-86.
. . .The relative lengths of the ischium and the thigh extending
muscles are compared in
macaques, the guereza, the chimpanzee, the gibbon,
the slow loris, and the grand and lesser bush babies.
Discussing the ischium in relation with the
hamstring muscles alone is not satisfactory, since the ischium
serves the thigh extending muscles, including
adductor magnus and quadratus femoris ventralis, attached on
it as a lever. A long lever, in these cases the ischium, is an
indication of the relatively heavy muscles that originate from it.
Animal Models
Tamarin model of pneumococcal bacteremia. Chudwin, D. S., Artrip,
S. G., Ogden, J. D. and Schiffman, G. (Dept. of
Immunology/Microbiology, Rush Medical College, 1753 West Congress
Parkway, Chicago, IL 60612.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16. 249-260.
. . .Tamarins
(Saguinus labiatus) were utilized to study host defenses
against pneumococcal bacteremia. Tamarins had a poor antibody response
to immunization with varying doses of pneumococcal capsular
polysaccharide (PCP) vaccine or to infection with
serotype 7F
Streptococcus pneumoniae. Tamarins were
protected against challenge with a lethal dose of serotype 7F
S. pneumoniae if the bacteria were preopsonized with human
immune globulin intravenous or if the tamarins were injected with the
immune globulin 30 min before challenge. There was minimal protection
utilizing a mouse monoclonal anti-type 7F PCP antibody.
Experimental American leishmaniasis in the Brazilian squirrel
monkey
(Saimiri sciureus): Lesions, hematology, cellular, and humoral
immune responses. Pung, O. J. and Kuhn, R. E. (Dept. of Biology,
Box 7325, Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem, NC 27109.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 165-174.
. . .Unulcerated cutaneous lesions appeared and persisted in
squirrel monkeys experimentally infected with
Leishmania braziliensis braziliensis or L. b. panamensis.
Peripheral blood mononuclear cell (PBMC) numbers increased following
infection, and cultured PBMCs from infected monkeys proliferated in
response to parasite antigens. The responses of PBMCs to mitogens
were not suppressed in infected monkeys. Elevated levels of
leishmania-specific immunoglobulins M and G were also observed. Thus,
the squirrel monkey is susceptible to American leishmaniasis and is
capable of responding to the infection with measurable cellular and
humoral immunity.
Hemodynamic and echocardiographic evaluation of the stumptailed
macaque: A potential nonhuman primate model for pulmonary vascular
disease. Weesner, K. M. and Kaplan, K. (Dept. of Pediatrics,
Comparative Medicine, Bowman Gray School of Medicine, Wake Forest
University, Winston-Salem, NC 27103.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 185-202.
. . .Stumptailed macaques
(Macaca arctoides) were evaluated as to their suitability as an
animal model for pulmonary hypertension.
Hemodynamic measurement including pulmonary artery
pressure response to 12% oxygen exposure identified three groups of
animals with elevated, normal, and intermediate pulmonary artery
pressures. Stumptailed macaques with elevated pulmonary artery
pressure could be distinguished from other animals by
echocardiography.
Behavior
Infanticide in a chacma baboon troop. Tarara, E. B. (Institute
of Primate Research, Box 24481, Karen, Nairobi, Kenya.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 267-270.
. . .This report describes an infanticide and two attacks of an
infant and a juvenile by a natal adult male in a troop of chacma
baboons (Papio ursinus). The infanticidal male had become the
dominant male in his troop five months before the infanticide,
suggesting that a trigger for infanticidal behavior is a rise in
rank to dominant status.
Rhythm and melody in gelada vocal exchanges. Richman, B. (2200
Oakdale, Cleveland Heights, OH 44118.)
Primates, 1987, >28, 199-223.
. . .A scheme for describing how gelada monkeys
(Theropithecus gelada) use rhythm and melody in their vocal
exchanges with each other is presented.
Many examples of gelada
rhythm and melody are demonstrated from sonagraphs. It is argued
that geladas use rhythm and melody in their vocal exchanges in ways
that are similar to how humans use rhythm and melody in speech and
singing.
Infanticide among free-ranging langurs
(Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur (Rajasthan/India): Recent
observations and a reconsideration of hypotheses. Sommer, V. (Dept.
of Zoology, University of Jodhpur, Jodhpur 342 002, Rajasthan, India.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 163-197.
. . .Detailed observations are
presented about two noninfanticidal and three infanticidal male
changes including six eye-witness and five presumed cases of
infanticide within three langur troops during a long-term study at
Jodhpur, Rajasthan, India. The results do not support any explanatory
hypotheses focussing on social crowding, regulation of population
density, social stress, sexual frustration, incest avoidance, or
social bonding, but are in general though not total agreement with the
reproductive advantage hypothesis.
The acquisition process of a novel behavior pattern in a group of
ring-tailed lemurs
(Lemur catta).
Kappeler, P. M. (Abt. für Verhaltensphysiologie, Beim
Kupferbammer 8, D 7400, Tübingen 1, DFR.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 225-228.
. . .The process of acquisition and propagation of a novel
behavior pattern in a group of 18
Lemur catta was examined. Six of eight animals that
acquired the new habit were infants or juveniles. Adult males did
not take up the new behavior pattern. Effects of rank and kinship
on the acquisition process were not obvious.
Social responses to the traumatic injury of a juvenile spider
monkey
(Ateles geoffroyi). Chapman, C. A. and Chapman, L. J. (Dept. of
Anthropology, University of Alberta, Edmonton, Alberta, T6G 2E1,
Canada.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 271-275.
. . .The behavioral development of a juvenile male spider monkey
preceding and subsequent to a traumatic injury resulting in the loss
of his tail, was documented over a nine-month period in Santa Rosa
National Park, Costa Rica. The juvenile became heavily reliant on his
mother, who readily provided compensatory care, including nursing the
juvenile even though he had previously been weaned. In comparison to
another juvenile male spider monkey of similar age, the development
of the injured animal was severely retarded and even five months after
the accident, the juvenile was still nursing and being carried by his
mother. As injuries may be relatively common among primates it is
suggested that the provision of compensatory care is an important
trait in many primate species.
A new interpretation of the social organization and mating system
of the callitrichidae. Sussman, R. W. and Garber, P. A. (Dept. of
Anthropology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri 63130.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 73-92.
. . .This paper
proposes that the Callitrichidae tend to live in small
multimale-multifemale groups, communally rear the young of one female,
and exhibit a mating pattern characterized by female promiscuity but
a breeding system that is functionally polyandrous. There is a
significant correlation between the number of males in a group and
the total number of surviving young. The possible adaptive advantages
of this social organization are discussed.
Ranging, activity rhythms, and sociality in free-ranging
Tarsius bancanus: A preliminary report. Crompton, R. H. and
Andau, P. M. (Dept. of Human Anatomy and Cell Biology, The University,
P. O. Box 147, Liverpool, L69 3BX, UK).
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 43-71.
. . .Two male and two female western tarsiers were followed in
primary and secondary lowland rainforest using radiotelemetry.
The present evidence suggests that the
social organization resembles that of Lorisinae and some Galaginae,
but with much less direct or close contact, and does not tend to
support arguments for the existence of monogamy or pair bonding in
this species.
Variability and stability in the rank relations of nonhuman
primate females: Analysis by computer simulation. Hausfater, G.,
Cairns, S. J., and Levin, R. N. (Division of Biological Sciences, 110
Tucker Hall, University of Missouri, Columbia, MO 65211.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 55-70.
. . .
The present analyses suggest that
differences between species in the degree of nepotism and
age-gradedness characteristic of their female rank orders does not
in and of itself constitute evidence that the females of those
species use markedly different strategies of rank acquisition.
Mirror guided behavior in Japanese monkeys
(Macaca fuscata fuscata). Itakura, S. (Primate Research
Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, 484 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 149-161.
. . .Two male Japanese monkeys were trained to use a mirror to reach
an object that could not be seen directly. Training to use a mirror
in this way proceeded, step-by-step, from reaching a piece of apple
to key-tracking.
Mirror guided behavior such as shown here
has not previously been demonstrated in monkeys.
Are male rhesus monkeys more aggressive than females? Reinhardt,
V. (Wisconsin Regional Primate Center, University of Wisconsin, 1223
Capitol Court, Madison, WI 53715-1299.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 123-125.
. . .The justification for the generalization that primate males
are more aggressive than females was tested in a troop of 24 captive
rhesus monkeys. Males were more dominant than females,
i.e. they had more subordinate partners,
hence, their aggression
rate was higher.
However, adjusting the rate of aggression according to the number of
subordinate targets revealed no gender difference and males directed
numbers of aggressive acts against individual subordinate partners that
were not higher than those of females.
It was concluded that aggressiveness is an
individual attribute that is (1) independent of dominance status, and
(2) independent of sex.
Sibships: Cooperation and competition among immature vervet
monkeys. Lee, P. C. (Sub-department of Animal Behaviour, Madingley,
Cambridge CB3 8AA, UK.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 47-59.
. . .The relationships between siblings were compared with those
between nonsibling peers among immature vervet monkeys
(Cercopithecus aethiops) in three free-ranging social groups.
Correlates of agonistic and competitive interactions in pregnant
baboons. Silk, J. B. (Dept. of Anthropology, University of California,
Los Angeles, CA 90024.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 479-495.
. . .
Recently, primatologists have suggested that certain
characteristics of the fetus may influence maternal condition.
For example, among captive pigtailed macaques
(Macaca nemestrina) mothers of female infants may be at greater
risk of injury during their pregnancies than mothers of male infants.
Analysis of the rates of aggression, submission, competition, and
wounding among free-ranging pregnant baboons
(Papio cynocephalus) in Amboseli National Park generally fail
to support these findings, but several other factors such as maternal
dominance rank, environmental conditions during pregnancy, maternal
parity, and fetal age correlate with aggression, submission,
competition, and injuries sustained by pregnant female baboons.
The manufacture and use of tools by capuchin monkeys
(Cebus apella). Westergaard, G. C. and Fragaszy, D. M. (CDMRC,
WJ-10, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195.)
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1987, 101, 159-168.
. . .
These studies provide the first report of
spontaneous manufacture of tools in any group of monkeys. The
tool-using and manufacturing behaviors observed in the capuchins were
similar in form, function, and ontogeny to those that have previously
been reported for chimpanzees.
Intra- and inter-group interactions of an all-male group of
Virunga mountain gorillas
(Gorilla gorilla beringei). Yamagiwa, J. (Japan Monkey Centre,
Inuyama, Aichi, 484 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 1-30.
. . .Six unrelated male gorillas formed an all-male group within
the Virunga mountain gorilla population. Frequent homosexual
interactions characterized the high cohesiveness of this group.
The group's formation may be related to recent social change
in the Virunga gorilla population.
Chimpanzees using stones to crack open oil palm nuts in Liberia.
Hannah, A. C. and McGrew, W. C. (Dept. of Psychology, University of
Stirling, Stirling FK9 4LA, Scotland.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 31-46.
. . .The use of stone-tools to open palm nuts
(Elaeis guineensis) was studied in a group of 16 chimpanzees
(Pan troglodytes) released from captivity to a natural island
setting in Liberia.
These are the first detailed behavioral data on
palm-nut-cracking, and they show many parallels with cracking of
other species of nuts
by chimpanzees of the Tai Forest, Ivory Coast. This is
another example of hammer-stone use from a limited region of West
Africa: southeastern Guinea, eastern Liberia, and western Ivory
Coast. This suggests limited cultural diffusion of the custom.
Biochemical Taxonomy
Origin and evolution of the Sulawesi macaques 1. Electrophoretic
analysis of hemoglobins. Takenaka, O., Hotta, M., Takenaka, A.,
Kawamoto, Y., Suryobroto, B. and Brotoisworo, E. (Primate Research
Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, 484 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 87-98.
. . .The monkeys on the island of Sulawesi (Celebes), Indonesia,
comprise seven species of
Macaca.
Hemoglobins from
248 individuals of these seven species were analyzed by isoelectric
focusing electrophoresis (IEFE) and by starch gel electrophoresis in
the presence of urea (USGE).
Origin and evolution of the Sulawesi macaques 2. Complete amino
acid sequences of seven beta chains of three molecular types. Takenaka,
O., Hotta, M., Kawamoto, Y., Suryobroto, B. and Brotoisworo, E.
(Primate Research Institute, Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, 484
Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 99-109.
. . .Seven beta chains were identified as the typical molecular types
carried by the seven species of Sulawesi macaques based on isoelectric
focusing and urea starch gel electrophoresis.
Banding patterns of the chromosomes of
Cebus apella: Comparative studies between specimens from
Paraguay and Argentina. Mudry De Pargament, M. and Slavutsky, I. R.
Sección Citogenética, Instituto de Investigaciones
Hematólogicas, Academia Nacional de Medicina, J. A. Pacheco de
Melo 3081 (1425), Buenos Aires, Argentina.
Primates, 1987, 28, 111-117.
. . .This paper presents a comparative study of the karyotypes of
Cebus apella (Platyrrhini, Cebidae) from two different
regions of South America: Paraguay and Argentina. Forty-two adult
animals were analyzed; similar karyotypes were observed in both
groups. Different chromosome aberrations were found in specimens from
Paraguay and Argentina. These findings support the intraspecific
variability of these New World monkeys.
Disease
Gastric trichobezoar in a baboon. Butler, T. M. and Haines, J. J.
Jr. (Dept. of Laboratory Animal Medicine, Southwest Foundation for
Biomedical Research, San Antonio, TX 78284.)
Laboratory Animal Science, 1987, 37, 232-233.
. . .A four year old, 9.4 kg female baboon was found in an outdoor
corral with a draining opening in the abdominal wall just distal to the
sternum. After a large firm mass was palpated, a laparatomy was
performed. The opening communicated directly with the gastric lumen
which contained a 1.0 kg trichobezoar. The gastric serosal surface
surrounding the opening was adhered to the abdominal wall. The hairball
was removed and the baboon made an uneventful recovery.
Tetanus in baboons of a corral breeding colony. Goodwin, W. J.,
Haines, R. J., and Bernal, J. C. (Dept. of Laboratory Animal Medicine,
Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research, San Antonio, TX 78284.)
Laboratory Animal Science, 1987, 37, 231-232.
. . .Tetanus caused by
Clostridium tetani is seldom a problem in nonhuman primates housed
in individual cages or in pens with solid floors that are cleaned
frequently. This organism is found in the soil and enters the body
primarily through wounds. Therefore, it is a potential health problem in
nonhuman primates being maintained on islands and in corrals. In 1982,
five cases of tetanus were diagnosed in baboons,
Papio cynocephalus anubis, of a 6 acre corral breeding colony
located at the Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research. To prevent
further cases, all animals were captured and vaccinated with veterinary
toxoid. Tetanus toxoid is now administered routinely to all young
animals annually, and a booster vaccination is given every 5 years.
Tetanus has not been diagnosed since.
Fatal fatty liver-kidney syndrome in obese monkeys. Laber-Laird, K.
E., Jokinen, M. P., and Lehner, N. D. M. (Dept. of Comparative Medicine,
Bowman Gray School of Medicine of Wake Forest University, Winston-Salem,
NC 27103.)
Laboratory Animal Science, 1987, 37, 205-209.
. . .A fatal syndrome associated with brief periods of anorexia or
acute weight loss was noted in monkeys. Affected monkeys died
unexpectedly or after a very short illness. Consistent gross findings at
necropsy were enlarged, pale yellow livers, pale tan to yellow kidneys,
abundant deposits of body fat and empty gastrointestinal tracts. Fatty
change of the liver and kidney was the predominant and characteristic
lesion found microscopically. Most of the monkeys were female
Macaca fascicularis greater than 8 years of age. However, monkeys
of other species and ages, and one male also died of this syndrome.
Affected monkeys generally had losses of 8-33% of body weight. In some
animals, this syndrome was associated with a location change forcing new
social interactions. A common clinical pathologic finding was azotemia.
Bovine tuberculosis in a wild baboon population: Epidemiological
aspects. Sapolsky, R. M. and Else, J. G. (Dept. of Biological
Science, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 229-235.
. . .A follow-up of an outbreak of
Mycobacterium bovis in a population of feral baboons in Kenya
was undertaken after one year by necropsy of euthanized, clinically
ill animals, and tuberculin testing of others. It was concluded that
the source of the infection was animals feeding on village
slaughterhouse offal of
M. bovis infected cows. Secondary (direct) baboon-baboon
transfer of the disease appeared to be minimal or nonexistent.
Sarcocystis found in the skeletal muscle of common squirrel
monkeys. Kimura, T., Ito, J., Suzuki, M. and Inokuchi, S. (Dept. of
Anatomy, Showa University School of Medicine, Shinagawa-ku, Tokyo,
142 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 247-255.
. . .Abnormally enlarged muscle fiber cells of ring form were
incidentally detected in transverse sections of muscles of the
common squirrel monkey during microscopic investigation of the
composition of muscle fibers. Detailed examination of longitudinal
sections revealed slender capsule-like
Sarcocystis cysts in the sarcoplasm.
Staining with Sudan black B revealed
that this parasite selectively infested type II white fibers with a
large diameter and a high glycogen content.
Mycobacterium paratuberculosis infection in a colony of
stumptail macaques
(Macaca arctoides). McClure, H. M., Chiodini, R. J., Anderson,
D. C., Swenson, R. B., Thayer, W. R. and Coutu, J. A. (Yerkes
Regional Primate Research Center, Emory University, Atlanta, GA
30322.)
The Journal of Infectious Diseases, 1987, 155, 1011-1019.
. . .Mycobacterium paratuberculosis infection was documented in
a colony of stumptail macaque monkeys with 29 (76%) of 38 monkeys
infected and shedding organisms in feces. Thirteen deaths have
occurred during the past five years.
The clinical and pathological
features of paratuberculosis in this species were comparable to
those reported for paratuberculosis in ruminants and
Mycobacterium avium infections in primates.
These findings extend the natural host range of
M. paratuberculosis to include nonhuman primates and add support
to current suggestions that
M. paratuberculosis may be pathogenic for humans.
Ecology
A field observation of predation on a moustached tamarin
(Saguinus mystax) by an anaconda.
Heymann, E. W. (AG Ethologie, Deutches Primatenzentrum, Kellnerweg 4,
3400 Göttingen, DFR.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 193-195.
. . .The first observation of snake predation on a New World
callitrichid monkey.
Responses of rain-forest primates to habitat disturbance: A review.
Johns, A. D. and Skorupa, J. P. (Ecology Graduate Group, Dept. of
Anthropology, University of California, Davis, CA 95616.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 157-191.
. . .Body size alone is a poor predictor of primate response to
moderate forest distrubance. However, when the effects of diet variables
are held constatn, body size more strongly correlates with survival
ability (smaller species surviving better). Degree of frugivory shows a
significant negative correlation with survival ability, while dietetic
diversity is not correlated with survival ability. Idiosyncratic
responses of species can usually be traced to specific features of the
changing environment, such as selective elimination of important food
sources and, conversely, the presence of increased densities of
particular food sources arising from the disturbance.
Notes on the distribution of
Lepilemur septentrionalis and L. mustelinus in northern
Madagascar. Ratsirarson, J., Anderson, J., Warter, S. and Rumpler, Y.
(Service des Eaux et Forêts, Tananarive, Madagascar.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 119-122.
. . .
This paper gives new information concerning the present southern
limits of
Lepilemur septentrionalis and northern limit of L. mustelinus.
This information is important in view of the habitat destruction and
human predation threatening the wildlife of Madagascar.
Comparability among measures of primate diets. Kurland, J. A.
and Gaulin, S. J. C. (Dept. of Anthropology, Pennsylvania State
University, University Park, PA 16802.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 71-77.
. . .Our picture of
primate feeding niches is suspect because field workers have used a
variety of observational techniques to assess diet in the wild. Here
the question of the comparability of these techniques is explored
empirically, by comparing the dietary profiles of a small group of
primate species that have been studied by two methods in a single
locality. These methods are shown to yield quite different results,
in both simple description and behavioral-ecological
hypothesis testing.
Ecological constraints on the grouping of wild orang-utans
(Pongo pygmaeus) in the Gunung Leuser National Park, Sumatra,
Indonesia. Sugardjito, J., te Boekhorst, I. J. A. and van Hooff,
J. A. R. A. M. (I. J. A. te Boekhorst, Laboratory of Comparative
Physiology, State University of Utrecht, Jan van Galenstraat 40,
3572 LA Utrecht, The Netherlands.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 17-41.
. . .The orangutan is an interesting subject on which to base an
evaluation of the costs and benefits of social life in apes, since
grouping in this heavy, arboreal frugivore is facultative. In the
Ketambe area of Sumatra, Indonesia, the food sources of wild
orangutans display a clear seasonality.
It is suggested that the development of social
skills is an important aspect of grouping in adolescent orangutans.
For the adult male, safeguarding his reproductive success by
protecting females against sexually aggressive subadult males is
probably the only reason for being in groups.
The surreptitious life of the saddle-backed tamarin. Terborgh, J.
and Stern, M. (Dept. of Biology, Princeton University, Princeton,
NJ 08544.)
American Scientist, 1987, 75, 260-269.
. . .The saddle-backed tamarin is a very small South American
monkey whose use of food resources may help us understand why some
species are plentiful and others rare. For ten months of the year,
tamarins are frugivores, harvesting fruit from a succession of small
trees rather than filling themselves at a few large trees as larger
primates do. During part of the dry season, the tamarins become
nectarivores, feeding on nectar from two plants. Because these two
critical resources occur in distinct forest types, the territories
of tamarins are larger and more heterogeneous than those of other
monkeys living in the same environment. The population density of
tamarins is consequently lower.
Immunology
Distribution of immunoglobulin allotypes among local populations
of Kenya olive baboons. Olivier, T. J., Coppenhaver, D. H. and
Steinberg, A. G. (Department of Anthropology, Yale University, 51
Hillhouse Ave., New Haven, CT 06511.)
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1986, 70, 29-38.
. . .This paper reports on the distributions of immunoglobulin
allotypes among 564 olive baboons collected at six localities in
Kenya.
The proportions of individuals positive for
polymorphic allotypes varied substantially between different local
samples, as did the arrays and estimated frequencies of haplotypes.
Allotype frequencies in local samples do not appear to be simply
related to either geographic location or habitat characteristics of
the localities. The data suggest that much of the geographic
variability in Kenya olive baboon populations occurs between
populations separated by small geographic distances.
Instruments and Techniques
Iliac biopsy for histomorphometric analysis of trabecular bone in
cynomolgus monkeys and baboons. Goodwin, B. T. and Jerome, C. P. (Dept.
of Comparative Medicine, Bowman Gray School of Medicine, Winston-Salem,
NC 27103.)
Laboratory Animal Science, 1987, 37, 213-216.
. . .A humane, repeatable surgical technique was developed to harvest
trabecular bone for histomorphometry from nonhuman primates. Using a
direct surgical approach to the iliac crest, bone specimens were
harvested from cynomolgus monkeys and baboons. The method provides bone
samples with little artifact or bone dust contamination, and easy
orientation and reproducibility of the plane of section. None of the
cynomolgus monkeys were affected adversely by the surgery, but the
baboons were quieter than normal for 12-24 hours afterward.
Nutrition
Failure to induce weight gain with palatable diets in monkeys
(Macaca mulatta). Jen, K.-L. C. (Dept. of Nutrition and Food
Science, Wayne State University, 160 Old Main, Detroit, MI 48202.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 61-69.
. . .Three diets popularly used to produce obesity in rodents were
offered to male rhesus monkeys. A high fat diet (fat: 50% of calories)
enhanced the daily caloric intake of the monkeys, but only slight and
nonsignificant increases in body weight were observed over a period
of six weeks. However, an increase in feeding efficiency was observed.
Providing monkeys with an assorted, palatable supermarket diet
failed to induce them to overeat. There were no changes in total
caloric intake or in body weight. When the monkeys were supplemented
with a bottle of 32% sucrose solution, in addition to a commercial
monkey biscuit and tap water, a significant increase in caloric
intake was observed, but no change in body weight occurred. Thus,
palatable and calorically dense diets failed to induce sufficient
increases in intake to produce change in body weight in nonhuman
primates. Based on these results, only the high fat diet has much
potential to produce obesity, and such obesity, if it occurs, will
likely require long-term experiments.
A preliminary list of chimpanzees' alimentation at Bossou,
Guinea. Sugiyama, Y. and Koman, J. (Primate Research Institute,
Kyoto University, Inuyama, Aichi, 484 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 133-147.
. . .A preliminary list of all the plant and animal foods for wild
chimpanzees at Bossou, southeast of the Republic of Guinea, is
presented for comparison with that of other study areas and as the
basic data for the further studies.
Altogether 205 food items (species x part eaten) from 156 species
in 106 genera of plant food are recorded.
As calculated from number of food items,
fruits and seeds (nuts) occupy more than half of the diet; and leaves
shoots, and twigs (stems) constitute about one third.
Evaluation of serum parameters relevant to vitamin D status in
tamarins. Flurer, C. I. and Zucker, H. (Institute für Physiologie,
Physiologische Chemie und Ernährungsphysiologie, Tierärztliche
Fakultät der Universität München, West Germany.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 175-184.
. . .Serum levels of 25(OH)D, alkaline phosphatase (AP), and
parathormone (PTH) were evaluated to investigate the vitamin D
requirement of
Saguinus fuscicollis. Diets with various vitamin D content were
fed 4 weeks and longer. The values of 25(OH)D (30-300 nmol/l), AP
(<300 U/l), and PTH (</=1,000 equl/l) considered as normal were
obtained with 2,000 IU
D3/kg diet, or 33 IU/animal/day, which we regard as the level
required. Animals depleted of vitamin D for 215 days developed a
secondary hyperparathyroidism.
Pathology
Oral-facial clefts and associated malformations in the squirrel
monkey
(Saimiri sciureus). Hoopes, C. W. and Jerome, C. P. (Department
of Comparative Medicine, Bowman Gray School of Medicine,
Winston-Salem, NC 27103.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 203-209.
. . .Of the 414 squirrel monkey pregnancies recorded at this
institution since 1977, seven (1.7%) have resulted in offspring with
clefts of the lip and/or palate. Associated malformations include a
ventricular septal defect, renal agenesis, anal atresia, axial
skeletal anomalies, and craniorachischsis (anencephaly and spina
bifida). Three of these infants are the result of consanguineous
matings.
Physiology
Separation distress in infant rhesus monkeys: effects of diazepam
and RO 15-1788. Kalin, N. H., Shelton, S. E., and Barksdale, C. M.
(William S. Middleton Memorial Veterans Hospital, Madison, WI 53705.)
Brain Research, 1987, 408, 192-198.
. . .This paper examines the effect
that altering benzodiazepine systems has on the behavioral and endocrine
response of infant rhesus monkeys to brief separation from their
mothers. The data support the hypothesis that in primates, endogenous
benzodiazepine systems modulate the behavioral and endocrine response to
the naturally occurring stress of separation.
Discriminant analysis of hematological and serum biochemical values
in cynomolgus monkey
(Macaca fascicularis) bred and reared under the indoor
individually-caged conditions. Yoshida, T., Cho, F., and Goto, N.
(Tsukuba Primate Center for Medical Science, National Institute of
Health Hachimandai, Yatabe-machi, Tsukuba-gun, Ibaragi-Ken 305, Japan.)
Experimental Animals, 1987, 36, 245-251. (Japanese, with English
summary)
. . .The data on hematological and serum biochimical properties of 1086
laboratory-bred cynomolgus monkeys in 10 age groups were analyzed by
discriminant analysis.
Comparative radionuclide and thermodilution determinations of
cardiac output and stroke volume in the baboon
(Papio ursinus). Dormehl, I. C., Bosman, H., Hugo, N., Maree, M.,
van Vuuren, C., van Zandwyk, C., van Aswegen, A. and Paterson, L.
(AEC Institute of Life Sciences, P.O. Box 2034, Pretoria, 0001,
South Africa.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 139-150.
. . .Thermodilution cardiac output determinations and multigated
equilibrium blood-pool scintigraphy were performed in ten healthy
chacma baboons. The correlation was moderately good between both
the radionuclide and thermodilution stroke volume (r = 0.58,
SEE = 3 ml; SV(th) = 0.78SV(r) + 15.6 ml) as well as the
cardiac output (r = 0.72, SEE = 0.2 liter/min; CO(th) = 0.56
Co(r) + 2.1 liter/min). The attenuation depth d(r) as determined by
radionuclide techniques was found to correlate well with the
radiologically determined values d(x) (r = 0.8, SEE = 0.4 cm;
d(x) = 0.87d(r) + 0.72 cm) which validated the depth values used
in the calculations.
Auditory frequency discrimination in primates: Species differences
(Cercopithecus, Macaca, Homo). Sinnott, J. M., Owren, M. J. and
Petersen, M. R. (Psychology Department, Indiana University,
Bloomington, IN 47405.)
Journal of Comparative Psychology, 1987, 101, 126-131.
. . .Auditory frequency difference limens (DLs) at 2 kHz were
measured in Old World monkeys and humans using a go/no-go repeating
standard procedure and positive reinforcement operant conditioning
techniques. Quantitative and qualitative differences occurred
between monkey and human sensitivity.
Results are related to other
comparative psychoacoustic data and primate vocal communication,
including human speech.
Reference values for peripheral blood lymphocytes from
Aotus lemurinus ssp. griseimembra (owl monkey). Caldwell, C.
W., Mrema, J. E. K., Taylor, H. M. and Green, T. J. (Dept. of
Pathology, University of Missouri School of Medicine, Columbia, MO
65212.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 261-171.
. . .The reactivities of several monoclonal antibodies that define
human lymphocyte cell-surface antigens have been tested with
peripheral blood lymphocytes of
Aotus lemurinus ssp. griseimembra. Based on reactivity
patterns in humans, reactive MoAb were identified that mark pan-T,
helper/inducer, suppressor/cytotoxic, pan-B, and natural killer
cells. Reference values of these subsets in
Aotus are presented. These MoAb should provide a useful tool for
further phenotypic and functional dissection of the immune system in
this simian model of human disease.
Correlative clinical biochemistry and hematological profiles of
laboratory-bred Bolivian squirrel monkeys
(Saimiri sciureus). Kakoma, I., James, M. A. , Jackson, W.,
Montealegre, F., Bennett, G., Carpunky, P. and Ristic, M. (Dept. of
Pathobiology, University of Illinois, College of Veterinary
Medicine, Urbana, IL 61801.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 273-276.
. . .Twenty-five clinical and biochemical parameters were
determined on 17 normal, male, laboratory-bred Bolivian squirrel
monkeys. Parallel hematologic parameters were conducted. The
correlation of biochemical and hematological observations and the
distribution characteristics, range, and standard error of the mean
were determined,
to establish baseline
biochemical values for laboratory-bred squirrel monkeys and to
present a correlative comparison between selected cellular elements
and major blood electrolytes.
Blood variables in adult stumptail macaques
(Macaca arctoides)
living in a captive group; Annual variability.
de Neef, K. J., Nieuwenhuijsen, K., Lammers, A. J. J. C., Degen, A. J.
M. and Verbon, F. (Medical Research and Development Unit, Organon
International B. V., P. O. B. 20, 5340 BH Oss, The Netherlands.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 237-247.
. . .Monthly blood samples were collected for more than two years
from adult stumptail macaques living in a large captive group. The
social organization, feeding pattern, and food composition were
stable throughout the study period. No seasonal variability was
observed for any of the 30 blood variables studied. It appeared that
for each variable, within-animal variance was small relative to
between-animal variance. A table of means and reference ranges is
presented and discussed.
Renin angiotensin aldosterone axis, including aldosterone
binding globulin and blood pressure in three species of nonhuman
primates. Mavoungou, D., Nowaczynski, W., Cooper, R. W., Collet,
J-Y. and Fung, K. (W. Nowaczynski, St. Paul's Hospital, 1081 Burrard
Street, Vancouver, B. C. V6Z 1Y6 Canada.
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 211-227.
. . .Variables of renin-angiotensin-aldosterone axis with inclusion
of protein binding to specific plasma globulin (ABG), plasma
cortisol, and the blood pressure (BP) were measured in 24 chimpanzees,
4 gorillas, and 16 cynomolgus monkeys. ABG activity was readily
detected in plasma from the primates. In chimpanzees and gorillas,
all the variables under baseline conditions were similar to those in
humans. In cynomolgus
(Macaca fascicularis), both the ABG binding capacity for
aldosterone and the diastolic or systolic BP were significantly higher
than in chimpanzees and
gorillas.
Reproduction
Early pregnancy diagnosis by the ultrasonographical device and
observation of fetal growth in cynomolgus monkeys
(Macaca fascicularis). Cho, F., Narita, H., Ono, T., and Honjo, S.
(Tsukuba Primate Center for Medical Science, National Institute of
Health, Hachimandai, Yatabe-machi, Tsukuba-gun, Ibaragi-ken, 305,
Japan.)
Experimental Animals, 1987, 36, 223-228. (Japanese, with English
summary)
. . .Usefulness of the ultrasonographical device for early diagnosis of
pregnancy and for observation of fetal growth was examined in the
cynomolgus monkey. Diagnosis was based on the detection of the
gestational sac in the uterus. It was possible to judge conditions of
fetal growth by measuring the size of the gestational sac and biparietal
diameter.
Measurement of excreted steroids in
Macaca nemestrina. Risler, L., Wasser, S. K. and Sackett, G. P.
(Samuel K. Wasser, Dept. of Psychology, University of
Washington, Seattle, WA 98195.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 91-100.
. . .A practical method for the quantitative measurement of the
estrogenic steroid estradiol-17beta in the feces of pigtailed
macaques
(Macaca nemestrina) is described.
These methods provide a
practical and accurate solution to the problem of collecting hormone
data in field studies without the potential complications of
capture in laboratory research.
Synchronization of ovarian cycles within and between social
groups in golden lion tamarins
(Leontopithecus rosalia). French, J. A. and Stribley, J. A.
(Dept. of Psychology, University of Nebraska, Omaha, NE
68182.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 469-478.
. . .
The temporal association of ovarian events was
examined in female golden lion tamarins living in the same or
different social groups. Ovarian cyclicity was monitored by
measuring the excretion of urinary estrogen metabolites. There was a
high degree of synchronization in the occurrence of urinary estrogen
peaks for females in different social groups (mean peak discrepancy
= 2.1 days) and in females housed in the same social group (mean peak
discrepancy = 1.3 days). Contrary to previous reports on callitrichid
primates, daughters housed in their natal family group exhibited
cyclic patterns of urinary estrogen excretion. These findings
represent the first explicit demonstration of ovarian synchrony in a
New World primate, and the tight coupling of ovarian cycles in female
tamarins resembles the nature of menstrual synchrony in human females.
Serum chorionic gonadotropin levels determined by
radioreceptorassay
and early diagnosis of pregnancy in the cynomolgus monkey
(Macaca fascicularis). Yoshida, T., Suzuki, K., Cho, F., and
Honjo, S. (Tsukuba Primate Center for Medical Sciences, National
Institute of Health, Hachimandai, Yatabe-machi, Tsukuba-gun
Ibaragi-Ken 305, Japan.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 101-106.
. . .The radioreceptorassay developed to determine serum luteinizing
hormone level in the cynomolgus monkey was evaluated for its
usefulness in early pregnancy diagnosis by the detection of serum
chorionic gonadotropin (CG). Blood samples were collected at weekly
intervals from the 1st to the 5th week after conception to determine
changes in circulating levels of CG. In the pregnancy cases, serum
CG levels increased to above 50 microg/ml in almost all animals.
95% of the pregnant cases were
detected by 4 weeks after conception, and 5% were undetected as
negative responses because their CG levels were low.
Birth intervals of
Cercopithecus monkeys of the Kakamega Forest, Kenya. Cords, M.
and Rowell, T. E. (Zoology Dept., University of California,
Berkeley, CA 94720.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 277-281.
. . .Data on interbirth intervals are reported for two forest-living
guenons,
Cercopithecus mitis and C. ascanius, from a western Kenyan
site. Measured intervals for females whose first offspring survived
varied from 24 to 54 months (median 47,
N = 10) for C. mitis, and from 49 to 60 months (median 52,
N = 3) for C. ascanius. Intervals were shorter when the first
of two offspring died. These results are supported by data on
estimated intervals, in which the date of the first of two births
was estimated, and incomplete intervals. Our measurements exceed
previous estimates of interbirth intervals in wild populations and
measured intervals of captive animals. Compared to closely related
species inhabiting unpredictable and seasonal environments, these
forest guenons breed very slowly indeed.
Acute effects of unilateral ovariectomy in the baboon
Papio cynocephalus. Shaikh, A. A., Gbur, E. E. and Shaikh, S. A.
(Dept. of Veterinary Physiology and Pharmacology, Texas A & M
University, College Station, TX 77843-4466.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 239-246.
. . .The purpose of this study was to determine if steroids secreted
by one ovary affected the steroid secretion of the other ovary by
direct transportation of the steroids via uterine blood vessels.
Statistical analysis showed that change in utero-ovarian vein or
uterine vein hormone levels on the contralateral side after removal
of one ovary was not significant for any of the four hormones
E&s'2 , P, T, and A. There is no evidence to demonstrate cross
circulation of steroids from one ovary to the other via direct
vascular channels.
Serum LH, progesterone and estradiol-17beta levels throughout
the ovarian cycle, during the early stage of pregnancy and after the
parturition and the abortion in the common marmoset
Callithrix jacchus. Torii, R., Koizumi, H., Tanioka, Y., Inaba,
T. and Mori, J. (Institute for Experimental Animals, Shiga University
of Medical Science, Seta, Ohtsu, Shiga, 520-21 Japan.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 229-238.
. . .In the ovarian cycle of common marmosets, serum progesterone
began to increase at two to three days after estradiol-17 beta or LH
surge, attained a peak of 25-70 ng/ml and then declined to a level
of under 2 ng/ml before the ensuing rise in estradiol-17 beta and LH.
Serum estradiol-17 beta increased to 700-5,500 pg/ml during the luteal
phase, synchronizing with progesterone. It is suggested that the
corpus luteum secreted estradiol-17 beta as well as progesterone. The
cycle length as determined from the interval between successive LH
surges was approximately 28 days. During the luteal phase, the
levels of progesterone and estradiol-17 beta were higher than in Old
World monkeys and women, but there were no
clinical symptoms due to excessive progesterone and estradiol-17 beta.
This suggests that such unresponsiveness to progesterone and
estradiol-17 beta in marmosets reflects the small amount of estradiol-17 beta
receptor and presumably also the lower function of the post receptor
system. Recovery of the post-partum ovarian cycle in two marmosets
differed from that observed in Old World monkeys and women.
This suggests that
the suckling stimulus of newborns in the common marmoset does not
cause any delay in recovery of the ovarian cycle. In three cases of
abortion, the recovery of the ovarian cycle was almost the same as
that in the case of normal parturition.
Sexual dimorphism and mating systems: Jumping to conclusions.
Rowell, T. E. and Chism, J. (Dept. of Zoology, University of
California, Berkeley, CA 94720.)
Human Evolution, 1986, 1, 215-219.
. . .Previous studies have suggested that there is a strong
relationship between a high degree of aggressive competition among
males for access to fertile females and large body and canine size
in males. It has further been suggested that such a relationship
among living primates can be used to infer the social organization of
extinct primate species from the degree of sexual dimorphism
exhibited.
It is suggested, on the basis of observations of
patas
(Erythrocebus patas) and blue monkeys (Cercopithecus mitis),
two species which had previously been characterized as having
one-male "harem" group structures,
that factors other than male-male competition (e.g.,
.hy add di-morph-ism
predation) may also have influenced the degree of dimorphism in
primates.
Determination of ovulation and pregnancy in the marmoset
(Callithrix jacchus) by monitoring of urinary hydroxypregnanolone
excretion. Heger, W. and Neubert, D. (Institut für Toxikologie und
Embryopharmakologie, Freie Universität Berlin, Garystr. 5, D-1000,
Berlin 33, FRG.)
Journal of Medical Primatology, 1987, 16, 151-164.
. . . In Callithrix jacchus the urinary excretion of
hydroxypregnanolone (5alpha-pregnane-3alpha, 7alpha-diol-20-one) shows a luteal
rise during the ovulatory cycle. This progesterone metabolite can
therefore be used as an indication of ovulation, implantation, and
subsequently for the persistence of the pregnancy. The excretion can
be monitored by high performance thin-layer chromatography
and postchromatographic derivatization after enzymatic hydrolysis of
the conjugate. Profiles of urinary hydroxypregnanolone levels are
presented and correlated with luteinizing hormone excretion. The
method is noninvasive and therefore suited for long-term studies
in these monkeys.
Testicular and serum testosterone variations in squirrel monkeys
during seasonal cyclicity. Pasqualini, T., Colillas, O. and Rivarola,
M. A. (Endocrinologia, Hospital de Ni&ntilda;os, Gallo 1330, 1425 Buenos
Aires, Argentina.)
Journal of Andrology, 1986, 7, 298-302.
. . .The seasonal testicular maturation of squirrel monkeys
(Saimiri sciureus) was used as a model of maturational hormonal
regulation of the testis. Testicular testosterone and serum
testosterone concentrations were determined during the circannual
variations of body weight and testicular volume. These data have been
correlated with changes in the germinal epithelium.
. . .Testicular testosterone showed two annual elevations. The first
peak
coincided with the serum testosterone peak when
body weight and testicular volume were high and the trophic response
of the germinal epithelium was complete. The second peak
was observed before the reinitiation
of spermatogenesis. This was accompanied by a moderate increase in
serum testosterone.
The second peak of testicular testosterone, which has been
reported to occur in the rat and in humans, might represent a local
androgen need for initiation of spermatogenesis, while the first
peak might represent the androgen need for full stimulation of
spermatogenesis.
Reproduction in the crowned lemur
(Lemur coronatus) in captivity. Kappeler, P. M. (Dept. of
Zoology, Duke University, Durham, NC 27706.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 497-503.
. . .Four pairs of captive crowned lemurs were studied during their
annual breeding season to obtain baseline data on their
reproductive biology for comparison with other
Lemur species and to enhance their captive breeding success.
Vaginal smears, testicular measurements, and records of the Duke
University Primate Center provided the presented data. During a single
breeding season, females cycled an average of three times, with an
average cycle length of 34 days. Cycles were detected between
November and March. Vaginal estrus and copulations were limited to one
day per cycle. After 125 days of gestation females gave birth to one
or two young. Both sexes attained sexual maturity at an age of about
20 months. Mean male testis size peaked in late December; at the
same time, three of the females experienced their first estrus. Based
on all available data, there was a significant positive correlation
between cycle length and gestation length in the genus
Lemur.
Supply
Conditioning and breeding facilities for the cynomolgus monkey
(Macaca fascicularis) in the Philippines: A progress report on
the SICONBREC project. Hobbs, K. R., Welshman, M. D., Nazareno, J. B.
and Resuello, R. G. (Shamrock Farms (Great Britain) Ltd, Victoria
House, Small Dole, Henfield, Sussex BN5 9XE, UK.)
Laboratory Animals, 1987, 21, 131-137.
. . .Large-scale breeding of
Macaca fascicularis in the country of origin is a way in which
the demands of the research community can be met while the feral
population of monkeys is safeguarded. This paper describes the
development of the Simian Conservation, Breeding and Research Centre
(SICONBREC) in the Philippines.
Taxonomy
The dentinoenamel junction in primates. Corruccini, R. S. (Dept. of
Anthropology, Southern Illinois Univeristy, Carbondale, IL 62901.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 99-114.
. . .Primate teeth were stripped of enamel after measurement, mapping,
and anatomical observations on the original crown. The dentin surfaces
thus revealed differ from the enamel surfaces in several respects and
shed new light on such dental problem areas as the origin of the
hypocone, the affinities of lorisoids and callitrichids, the monophyly
of the anthropoid grade, and human affinities.
Reproductive strategies, body size, and encephalization in primate
evolution. Shea, B. T. (Dept. of Anthropology, Northwestern University,
Evanston, IL 60201.)
International Journal of Primatology, 1987, 8, 139-156.
. . .The possible relationships among encephalization, precociality,
small size, and arboreality are discussed, particularly in light of
recent debates concerning the validity of the superorder Archonta. This
work emphasizes that we need to consider relative brain size as but one
element in a complex synergistic network of morphological and
life-history features.
Positional behavior of female Bornean orangutans
(Pongo pygmaeus). Cant, J. G. H. (Dept. of Anatomy, School of
Medicine, University of Puerto Rico, G. P. O. Box 5067, San Juan,
PR 00936.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 71-90.
. . .
The orangutan is by far the largest mammal that travels
in forest canopy, and a consideration of the ways that its
positional behavior solves problems posed by habitat structure,
particularly the tapering of branches and gaps between trees,
indicates that suspensory capacities have been essential in
permitting the evolution and maintenance of its great body size.
Multivariate sexing of the viscercranium in
Cercopithecus pygerythrus. Kieser, J. A. and Groeneveld, H. T.
(Dept. of Anatomy, Witwatersrand University Medical School,
Johannesburg, South Africa.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 127-131.
. . .One hundred
C. pygerythrus crania were subjected to canonical and
stepwise discriminant analyses utilizing four viscerocranial
measurements and two indices to determine specific characteristics
useful in sexual distinction of skeletal material. The
maxillo-alveolar length alone accounted for correct classification of
96.33% of males and 93.75% of females. Addition of the bimaxillary
breadth enables 98.17% of males and 95.83% of females to be
correctly assigned. From the observed contrast between univariate and
multivariate analyses of the data set, it is concluded that both
methods should routinely be combined in the evaluation of skeletal
material.
The phylogenetic and taxonomic status of
Pan paniscus: A chromosomal perspective. Stanyon, R., Chiarelli,
B., Gottlieb, K. and Patton, W. H. (Institute of Anthropology,
University of Pisa, 56100 Pisa, Italy.)
American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 1986, 69, 489-498.
. . .
In this report the
phylogenetic position and taxonomic status of the pygmy chimpanzee is
examined by a three-way comparison of the banded chromosomes of
humans, Pan troglodytes and P. paniscus. The evaluation
pays particular attention to distinguishing ancestral versus derived
chromosomes in order to determine the direction of the chromosomal
differences found between these three taxa.
P. paniscus and P. troglodytes are shown to share a number of
derived chromosomal changes indicative of a probable extensive period
of common evolution after the divergence of the humans. The
karyological evidence also demonstrates that the pygmy chimpanzee is
most likely a valid species, and that it is also the most chomosomally
specialized of the three taxa examined. Therefore, from the
chromosomal perspective, the pygmy chimpanzee is not the best model
for the last common ancestor of both humans and African apes. The
chromosomal data also suggest that all the African apes have evolved
more from this last common ancestor than is generally considered.
Hominoid nasal region polymorphism and its phylogenetic
significance. Eckhardt, R. B. (Dept. of Anthropology and Graduate
Program in Genetics, State University Park, PA 16802.)
Nature, 1987, 328, 333-335.
. . .The morphology of the nasal bones and their articulations with
adjoining frontal and maxillary bones have recently been reported in
Nature and elsewhere to be diagnostic of hominoid taxa, and
cladistic analysis based on these features has been used to assign
two immature Plio-Pleistocene hominoids (AL 333-105 and Taung) to
different lineages
(Paranthropus and Homo, respectively). Because earlier
studies had established that hominoid crania are highly variable
intraspecifically, it seemed desirable to try to replicate the study
reporting a consistently different pattern for each hominoid taxon.
Results show extensive polymorphism in the nasal region within every
taxon of extant pongids, including several rather clear examples, in
extant Pan troglodytes, of that recently hypothesized to be a
'parathropine' pattern. These findings underscore the substantial
risks inherent in cladistic analyses using very restricted character
sets to assign individual specimens to particular taxa.
Type locality of
Hylobates concolor leucogenys. Fooden, J. (Div. of Mammals,
Field Museum of Natural History, Roosevelt Road at Lake Shore Drive,
Chicago, IL 60605.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 107-110.
. . .Kloss (1929) restricted the type locality of the northern
white-cheeked gibbon on the basis of two specimens that were
collected at Muang Khi, Laos, not at Muang Pak-Lay, Laos, as
conventionally assumed. The locality difference is zoogeographically
important because Muang Khi is east of the Mekong River, within the
known range of
H. c. leucogenys, whereas Muang Pak-Lay is west of the river,
outside the known range of the subspecies. The type-locality
restriction is appropriately amended.
Uacaries, New World monkeys of the genus
Cacajao (Cebidae, Platyrrhini): A preliminary taxonomic review
with the description of a new subspecies. Hershkovitz, P. (Field
Museum of Natural History, Chicago, IL 60605.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 1-53.
. . .The two known species of uacaries, inhabitants of the upper
Amazonian region, are the black head
Cacajao melanocephalus with subspecies C. m. melanocephalus
Humboldt and
C. m. ouakary Spix, and the larger bald head uacari C. calvus
with subspecies
C. c. ucayalii Thomas, C. c. rubicundus I. Geoffroy and
Deville,
C. c. calvus I. Geoffroy, and C. c. novaesi described as new.
The species are described
and compared and their geographic distribution plotted with those of
their subspecies delimited. Sexual differences are outlined.
A gazetteer identifies all locality
records plotted by numbers on the geographic distribution maps.
Revision of the subspecies of
Australopithecus africanus (Primates: Hominidae), including a
new subspecies from the late Pliocene of Ethiopia. Ferguson, W. W.
(Dept. of Zoology, Tel Aviv University, Ramat Aviv, 69 978, Tel
Aviv, Israel.)
Primates, 1987, 28, 258-265.
. . .The subspecies of
Australopithecus africanus Dart, 1925 have been revised in a
morphological and statistical analysis. Four subspecific names were
previously proposed, but only one was found to be valid. The
subspecies
A. africanus transvaalensis (Broom, 1936), from the
Plio/Pleistocene of South Africa, cannot be sustained due to an
insufficient sample, and is combined with the nominate race,
A. a. africanus. The type of A. africanus afarensis Tobias,
1980 is a mistake in identification and not
A. africanus, but a pongid. The population of A. africanus
from the late Pliocene of Ethiopia does indeed represent a relatively
small-toothed geographical race for which the name
A. africanus aethiopicus was conditionally proposed; and the
lectotype for it, A.L. 288-1, is not
A. africanus, but the type of Homo antiquus Ferguson, 1984.
The trinominal
aethiopicus is thus unavailable for the Ethiopian race, which
is redescribed as a new subspecies,
A. africanus miodentatus n. ssp., and the mandible A.L. 266-1 is
designated as the holotype.
The taxonomy of South American sakis, genus
Pithecia (Cebidae, Platyrrhini): A preliminary report and
critical review with the description of a new species and a new
subspecies. Hershkovitz, P. (Field Museum of Natural History,
Roosevelt Road at Lake Shore Drive, Chicago, IL 60605.)
American Journal of Primatology, 1987, 12, 387-468.
. . .
The species of
Pitecia are arranged in the Pithecia pithecia group with
P. p. pithecia and P. p. chrysocephala, and the Pithecia
monachus group with P. m. monachus, P. m. milleri, P. irrorata
irrorata, P. i. vanzolinii (new subspecies), P. aequatorialis
(new species), and
P albicans. The discussion on nomenclature compares the usage of
names by various authors for the different taxa. The species
previously identified by Hershkovitz as
Pithecia monachus proves to be the new species P.
aequatorialis, whereas the sympatric species he called
P. hirsuta Spix (1823) is the same as true P. monachus
É. Geoffroy (1812).
Details of sexual dimorphism in
coloration and pelage patterns are described, and those of size are
documented by summaries of weights and by measurements of external,
cranial, dental, and skeletal characters for each sex of each taxon.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
In many cases, the original source of references in this section has been the Current Primate References prepared by The Primate Information Center, Regional Primate Research Center, Regional Primate Research Center SJ-50, University of Washington, Seattle, WA 98195. Because of this excellent source of references, the present section is devoted primarily to presentation of abstracts of articles of practical or general interest. In most cases, abstracts are those of the authors.
-------------------------------------------------------------------
* * *
In Volume 26, number 3, of this Newsletter, the author's name, Woodrow W. Denham, was inadverdently left out of the review of West Indian Green Monkeys: Problems in Historical Biogeography. Contributions to Primatology, Volume 24.
* * *
All correspondence concerning the Newsletter should be addressed to:
Judith E. Schrier, Psychology Department, Box 1853, Brown University
Providence, Rhode Island 02912. (Phone: 401-863-2511)
Judith_Schrier@brown.edu
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The Newsletter is supported by U. S. Public Health
Service Grant RR-00419 from the Animal Resources Program,
Division of Research Resources, N.I.H.
We are grateful to Linda Straw Coelho of San Antonio, Texas for providing the cover drawing of a ringtailed lemur, Lemur catta.
Dr. James Harper, Director of the Brown University Animal Care Facility, is now acting as an additional Consulting Editor on matters of laboratory animal science.
Copyright @1987 by Brown University
Editor: Allan M. Schrier
Associate Editor: Judith E. Schrier
Consulting Editor: Morris L. Povar
Managing Editor: Millicent Moverman